Diversity & Equality in Health and Care Open Access

  • ISSN: 2049-5471
  • Journal h-index: 10
  • Journal CiteScore: 3.5
  • Journal Impact Factor: 4.4
  • Average acceptance to publication time (5-7 days)
  • Average article processing time (30-45 days) Less than 5 volumes 30 days
    8 - 9 volumes 40 days
    10 and more volumes 45 days
Reach us +32 25889658

Research Article - (2018) Volume 15, Issue 5

Comparative use of Tuskegee Syphilis Study Film vs. Text Triggers to Teach Bioethics: The Spheres of Ethics Teaching Using Film (SOETUF) College Study

Ralph V Katz1*, Amos E Katz2, Rueben C Warren3, Monnica T Williams4, Hala Aqel5, Daniil Ilin6 and Richard McGowan7

1NYU Abu Dhabi Social Sciences Program and NYU College of Dentistry, UAE

2NYU Abu Dhabi Film Program, UAE

3National Center for Bioethics in Research and Health Care at Tuskegee University, US

4Psychological Sciences Department, University of Connecticut, US

5NYU Abu Dhabi Social Sciences Program, UAE

6NYU Tisch School of the Arts, UAE

7NYU College of Dentistry and NYU Library Sciences Program, US

Corresponding Author:

Ralph V Katz
Professor, Department of Epidemiology & Health Promotion
NYU College of Dentistry. Rm 706 433 1st Avenue
New York, NY 10010, USA
Tel: 001-212-253-2292
E-mail: ralph.katz@nyu.edu

Received Date: August 07, 2018 Accepted Date: August 28, 2018 Published Date: September 05, 2018

Visit for more related articles at Diversity & Equality in Health and Care

Abstract

The overall goal of this study was to determine whether one could demonstrate a measurable difference between the use of film vs. text exposure triggers-using documentary materials about the infamous Tuskegee Syphilis Study-in the teaching bioethics to college students using the 36-item ‘Spheres of Ethics Teaching Using Film’ Post-Trigger (SOETUF P-T) Questionnaire, as developed for this study. The results revealed statistically significant differences between the film vs. text groups for the overall Principles of Bioethics Domainof- Interest (7.3±4.4 vs. 5.1±4.4, p <0.05) with the film group scoring higher and for the 23-item ‘Emotional Reaction Domain-of-Interest’ (ER-DOI) with two of the five emotional components identified by a Principle Component Analysis (PCA), the components of being Content and Aroused, but not for the other three identified components of Unhappy, Afraid or Excited. Moreover, the SOETUF P-T Questionnaire items appeared to have reasonable face validity as revealed by the answer patterns to its range of items. In this study, the SOETUF P-T Questionnaire demonstrated that it could detect meaningful differences in student reaction to a film trigger versus a text trigger about the Tuskegee Syphilis Study in the teaching of bioethics to college students.

Key Points

What is known:

1) Bioethics teaching is critical in medical education use of film engages students

2) Tuskegee Syphilis Study (1932-1972) was worst research abuse in U.S.

3) Qualitative studies show that use of film in education enhances study learning

What this paper adds:

1) Provides quantitative evidence of that use of film enhances student learning in bioethics

2) Provides evidence of a value of a new research instrument: the SOETUF P-T Questionnaire

3) Identifies specifically how film enhances learning in bioethics

Keywords

Bioethics; Education; Biomedical; Teaching methods

Introduction

The medical education literature presents many articles and studies that discuss the value and the importance of using film in teaching bioethics to a variety of health care student groups from around the globe [1-15]. Angelo E Volandes, medical doctor from Massachusetts General Hospital points out that although written or verbal vignettes are useful as a pedagogic tool for teaching ethics and introducing students to real cases, they are limited, since students must imagine the clinical scenario…[while] Film vignettes fill in that imaginative leap. By providing vivid details with images, film vignettes offer rich and textured details of cases, including the patient’s perspective and the clinical reality [14]. Moreover, according to Sabine Woehlke and Silke Schicktanz, “movies can be a wonderful starting point to teach bioethics (as)… Teachers can use these popular narratives for bioethical reflection: they provide useful, compelling, and even “cool” case studies for bioethical issues and give fleshed-out interpretations of bioethical claims” [15].

A study proving the same idea that film fills that visual gap missing from reading material is missing was conducted in Bulgaria, used 92 students in management of healthcare to see whether they find any benefit from watching movies when it comes to teaching ethics and concluded that “the introduction of movies in the courses of bioethics had the potential to provide vivid illustrations of bioethical issues and to contribute to the exploration of specific theses and arguments” [2]. Following the same idea, Stanford Center for Biomedical Ethics has a whole program that uses films in order to teach biomedical ethics-“founded in 1998 by award-winning filmmaker and physician, Maren Grainger Monsen, the Program in Bioethics and Film creates films and education programs that touch people emotionally and intellectually to cause them to think deeply about important issues in healthcare and improve care” [14].

This project, the Spheres of Ethics Teaching Using Film (SOETUF), is the first study that approaches the question of what quantified differences, if any, result from use a of film trigger versus a text trigger in the teaching of bioethics. The study looks at how both the informational and emotional content of books and films, and the difference in the story/ information-telling techniques of the two mediums, affect the way students perceive and digest the material presented to them. While teaching medicine, bioethics in particular, using film has been advocated by other people for over 40 years, and the term “cinemeducation” was invented to describe this concept, there has been no cinemeducation study that has directly measuredand quantified-the impact of exposure to film triggers versus text triggers on students’ emotional reactions nor on the learning of bioethical principles [1,16,17].

The overall goal of this study was to determine whether there is a measurable impact difference between the use of film vs. text-using documentary material about the Tuskegee Syphilis Study-in the teaching bioethics to college students. The specific research questions addressed to answer this overall goal were whether there were any differences in learning outcome between students exposed to a trigger film versus a trigger text on bioethical issues regarding either the impact on the students’ emotional reactions or on the bioethical conclusions drawn by the students.

Methods

The research team developed the ‘Spheres of Ethics Teaching Using Film’ Post-Trigger (SOETUF P-T) Questionnaire for use in this SOETUF Pilot Study to be used after subjects were exposed to a ‘bioethics trigger’, i.e., either a text or a film that presented bioethical issues about the infamous Tuskegee Syphilis Study. This primary research instrument, the SOETUF P-T Questionnaire, was created to determine whether film versus text triggers that addressed bioethical issues evoked different emotional and/or cognitive responses in student subjects.

The final SOETUF P-T Questionnaire consisted of 36 items in total, including three domains-of-interest (DOI): 1) the 23-item ‘Emotional Reaction Domain-of-Interest’ (ER-DOI); 2) the 8-item ‘Principles of Bioethics Domain-of-Interest (POB-DOI); 3) the 3-item ‘Emotional Power, Morality Sense and Justice Sense Domain-of-Interest (EMJ-DOI); and, 4) two demographic items, i.e., age and sex (Appendix A for 36-item SOETUF P-T Questionnaire).

The primary specific aims of this proposed study were to determine whether there are measurable impact differences between the use of film versus text in the teaching of bioethics to college students using the ‘Spheres of Ethics Teaching Using Film’ Post-Trigger (SOETUF P-T) Questionnaire as measured in college students by the Emotional Reaction Domain of Interest (ER-DOI) Scale or the Principles of Bioethics Domain of Interest (POB-DOI) Scale. The secondary specific aim was to describe any differences in the primary specific aims by se.

The study consisted of New York University at Abu Dhabi (NYUAD) college student volunteers who took the SOETUF P-T Questionnaire after being exposed to one of the two trigger exposures about the USPHS Syphilis Study at Tuskegee (a.k.a., the Tuskegee Syphilis Study): 1) one group (n=41) having viewed the 1991, 52-minute documentary film ‘Bad Blood’ by Diverse Productions, Ltd. (Bad Blood by Diverse Productions Ltd, 1992), and the other group (n=42) having read (in ~ 45- 50 minutes) a slightly edited 1978 printed article describing the USPHS Syphilis Study at Tuskegee by Allan Brandt as published in the Hasting Report Journal [17-20]. All the data were collected in two separate data collection days in the Spring of 2017 in the Social Science Experimental (SSEL) Lab at the NYU Abu Dhabi campus, a computer research facility where 24 computer stations which were loaded with the trigger films as well as the SOETUF P-T on each of the two data collection days. The SSEL Technical and Operational Manager used their SSEL Lab master list of NYUAD college student research volunteers to select a sex balanced set of research subjects for each invited study group. The subjects were each reimbursed at the rate of 150 AED (Emirati dirhams, ~$40.55 U.S.) for their 1.5 hours of participation which included viewing the trigger and completing the SOETUF P-T Questionnaire. For this first use of the SOETUF Questionnaire, a sample size of 85 total students was targeted, based upon the maximal use of the available research funds.

When viewing the trigger film ‘Bad Blood’ the student volunteers used high quality head-phones for the audio aspect and viewed the film on computer video display of 1500*844 pixels on high quality monitors with a maximum resolution quality of 1920*1200 pixels. The films were shown with a video aspect ratio of 16:9, as universally used for high definition television as well as for YouTube display of movies.

The SSEL staff developed and maintained computer screen formatted questionnaires for the SOETUF P-T Questionnaire and delivered the data in Excel files from each of the completed data collection sessions within a 2-week period by a secure means. These data were then de-identified and converted into SPSS (v24) files. The study required written informed consent and was approved by the NYUAD IRB as an Expedited Review category study.

The primary statistical analysis for each of the two primary specific aims used ANOVA on the mean scores of the respective two Domains of Interest (the ER-DOI and the POB-DOI) between the group exposed to the film trigger vs. the group exposed to the text trigger. For all 8 items in POB-DOI that were retained for the final analysis, scores were reversed, as needed, to ensure that on the 5-point 0-4 scale for each time, a score of 4 meant ‘highest fulfillment’ of that bioethical principle and a score of 0 meant ‘lowest fulfillment’ of that bioethical principle. ANOVA analysis was also used to study the effect of sex on the two primary specific aims. In addition, a Principal Component Analysis (PCA) was used to identify the principal components within the 23-items of the ‘Emotional Reaction Domain-of-Interest’ (ER-DOI). All analyses were conducted using SPSS v24.

Results

The age and sex distribution of the 83 volunteer college students from the NYU Abu Dhabi baccalaureate program who participated as subjects were similar for each trigger exposure group, with the film trigger group (n=41) having a mean age of 20.8 (± 1.6) years with 46.3% being female and the text trigger group (n=42) having a mean age of 21.0 (± 1.7) years with 50.0% being female.

The mean POB-DOI scores for the film exposure trigger group vs. the text exposure trigger group for the overall POD-DOI and for each its eight component items grouped within the four principles of bioethics, i.e., autonomy, beneficence, non-maleficence, and justice (Table 1). As can be seen, the film exposure trigger group scored higher than the text exposure trigger group on the overall POB-DOI (7.3 ± 4.4 vs. 5.1 ± 4.4, p<0.05) on a scale of 0-32. As can also be seen, this statistically significance difference was largely due to the observed difference on the two beneficence subset items (where the differences observed achieved statistical significance or borderline statistical significance), although-for each of the other six subset items on autonomy, non-maleficence and justice-the universal pattern showed a higher (albeit nonstatistically significant) POB-DOI score for the film exposure trigger group.

Bioethics Principles             Trigger Group Mean score (± s.d.)     Eta 
Autonomy #1
(range 0-4)1
Film Exposure
Text Exposure
1.4 (± 1.4)
0.9 (± 1.3)
0.19
Autonomy #2
(range 0-4)           
Film Exposure
Text Exposure
0.9 (± 1.2)
0.7 (± 1.1)
0.10
Beneficence #2
(range 0-4)
Film Exposure
Text Exposure
0.7 (± 1.2)*
0.2 (± 0.6)*
0.29
Beneficence #2
(range 0-4)
Film Exposure
Text Exposure
1.2 (± 1.3)**
0.7 (± 1.0)**
0.21
Non-maleficence #1
(range 0-4)   
Film Exposure
Text Exposure
0.4 (± 0.7)
0.3 (± 0.7)
0.08
Non-maleficence #2
(range 0-4)
Film Exposure
Text Exposure
1.3 (± 1.3)
1.2 (± 1.3              
0.02
Justice #1
(range 0-4)           
Film Exposure
Text Exposure
0.2 (± 0.6)
0.1 (± 0.5)
0.05
Justice #2
(range 0-4)
Film Exposure
Text Exposure
1.1 (± 1.5)
0.9 (± 1.4)
0.07
TOTAL POB-DOI
Scale Scores
(Range: 0 – 32)2  
Film Exposure
Text Exposure
7.3 (± 4.4)***
5.1 (± 4.4)***
0 .24  

1scoring scale 0-4: 4=highest Bioethics Principles with 0=lowest Bioethics Principles
2scoring scale 0-32: 32=highest Bioethics Principles with 0=lowest Bioethics Principles
*statistically significant at p=0.01
**borderline statistically significant at p=0.055
***statistically significant at p=0.03

Table 1: Comparison of the POB (Principles of Bioethics)-DOI mean scores1,2 between the college students exposed to the film trigger vs. exposed to the text trigger for the total POB Scale and each its eight component items.

The data reveal that while exposure to either the film trigger or the text trigger resulted in equally high overall EMJ-DOI scores (10.0 ± 2.3 vs. 9.9 ± 2.0) on a scale of 0-12, there were also no differences detected between the film exposure trigger group and the text exposure trigger group for any of the component parts of the EJM-DOI, i.e., neither the emotional power of the story on them, nor the arousing of a sense of injustice, nor a violation of their sense of morality (Table 2).

Trigger Exposure Emotional power of the story (± s.d.) Sense of injustice (± s.d.) Sense of morality (± s.d.)
Film Exposure (n=41) 3.2(± 0.9)               3.4(± 0.8) 3.4(± 0.9)  
Text Exposure (n=42)
p value  
3.1(± 0.8)              
p=0.53
3.4(± 0.8)
p = 0.85
3.5(± 0.7)
p=0.83

1scoring scale of 0-4:0=weakest feeling/sense, and 4=strongest feeling/sense

Table 2: Comparison of mean EMJ-DOI scores1 for emotional power of the story, sense of injustice and sense of morality between the college students exposed to the film trigger vs. exposed to the text trigger.

The comparison of the 23-items in the Emotional Reaction Domain of Interest (ER-DOI) component of the SOETUF P-T Questionnaire for the film exposure trigger vs. the text exposure trigger groups (Table 3). One major observation on the data from these twenty-three emotion items is that, on the 5-point scale used, the nine emotions of frustrated, sadness, disturbed, anger, irritated, worried, uncomfortable, pessimistic and hopeless scored high (i.e., a mean score between 2.4-4), while another set of seven emotions (such as pleasure, joy, happiness, optimistic, energetic, aroused, and satisfied) scored very low, i.e., a mean score 0.1-0.6. The remaining seven emotions of anxious, intimidated, scared, fearful, terrified, frightened and threatened had mean scores between 1.3 and 1.9.

Emotion Film Trigger (± s.d.)     Text Trigger (±s.d.)    Statistical Significance
Stronger for FILM trigger:  
Hopeless               2.4(±1.1) 1.8(±1.3) p<0.05
satisfied 0.3(±0.6) 0.1(±0.3) p<0.05
frustrated 3.2(±0.9) 2.8(±1.3) borderline*
sadness  3.1(±1.0) 2.7(±1,2) borderline*
worried 2.8(±1.0) 2.3(±1.3) borderline*
Stronger for TEXT trigger:  
aroused 0.2(±0.5) 0.7(±1.0) p<0.01  
energetic 0.4(±0.7) 0.7(±0.9) p <0.05
No statistically significant difference between FILM vs TEXT triggers: (ranked by strength felt)  
disturbed 3.1(±1.0)                3.1(± 0.8) n.s.
anger 3.0(±1.1) 2.7(± 1.3) n.s.
irritated  2.9(±1.1) 2.7(± 1.3) n.s.
uncomfortable                2.6(±1.1) 2.4(± 1.1) n.s.
pessimistic 2.6(±1.2) 2.5(± 1.2) n.s.
anxious 1.9(±1.2) 1.9(± 1.4) n.s.
intimidated 1.8(±1.4) 1.3(± 1.3) n.s.
scared 1.7(±1.3) 1.5(± 1.3)                n.s.
fearful 1.6(±1.3) 1.6(±1.3) n.s.
terrified  1.4(±1.3)                1.4(±1.3) n.s.
frightened 1.3(±1.3) 1.7(±1.4) n.s.
threatened 1.3(±1.3) 1.4(±1.4) n.s.
optimistic 0.6(±0.9) 0.5(±0.8) n.s.
happiness 0.2(±0.5) 0.1(±0.4) n.s.
joy 0.1(±0.2) 0.0(±0.2) n.s.
pleasure    0.1(±0.3) 0.0(±0.2) n.s.

1scoring scale: 4=very strong
3=strong
2=moderate
1=weak
0=not at all

Table 3: Comparison of mean scores1 for the 23 emotions between subjects exposed to the film trigger vs. exposed to the text trigger on the SOETUF P-T Questionnaire.

The analysis of the 23 emotions comprising the ER-DOI for the film exposure trigger group vs. the text exposure trigger group revealed that for five emotions, the film exposure trigger group had higher mean scores, with two of them-the emotions of hopeless (2.1 vs. 1.8, respectively) and of satisfied (0.3 ± 0.6 vs. 0.1 ± 0.3, respectively)-being statistically significant at p>0.05, while three emotions (frustrated, sadness and worried) achieved borderline significance (a p-value between 0.06-0.10) with scores of 3.2 ± 0.9 vs. 2.8 ± 1.3, 3.1 ± 1.0 vs. 2.7±1.2 and 2.8 ± 1.0 vs. 2.3 ± 1.3 for the film vs text triggers, respectively) (Table 3). Conversely, two emotions-aroused and energetic-had statistically significant higher mean scores for the text exposure trigger vs. the film exposure trigger (0.7 ± 1.0 vs. 0.2 ± 0.5 and 0.7 ± 0.9 vs. 0.4 ± 0.7, respectively). For the other 16 emotions, the data show virtually no numerical (much less statistically significant) differences reported by the film vs. text exposure trigger groups.

Overall, for all 83 subjects (i.e., combining all subjects for both trigger exposures) only 3 of 23 emotions showed a statistically significant difference (at p>0.05) by sex. Males indicated a stronger emotional score for ‘satisfied’ (0.3 ± 0.6 vs. 0.1 ± 0.3) and ‘happiness’ (0.2 0.5 vs. 0.1 ± 02) although both sexes were at the extreme low end of the 0-4 scale on these two emotions. For the third emotion with a statistically significant gender difference, females indicated a stronger emotional score on ‘worried’ (2.9 ± 1.2 vs. 2.2 ± 1.1).

The final statistical analysis of these 23 emotions consisted of a Principal Component Analysis (PCA) for the mean scores for the combined film + text exposure trigger groups (n=83) to identify clustered subsets of these 23 emotion terms (Tables 4 and 5). As the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) Test for Sampling Accuracy rated the sample at .83 (i.e., a good rating level), and the data set passed the Bartlett’s Test, the PCA analysis proved to be robust. The PCA identified 5 components using the Varimax with Kaiser Normalization Rotation Method with the rotation converged in 6 iterations with a cut-off point set at 0.50 for component loadings (Table 4). Component 1 consisted of eight emotions (in descending order: frustrated, sadness, anger, disturbed, worried, irritated, uncomfortable and pessimistic) and was labeled Unhappy. Component 2 also consisted of eight emotions (fearful, scared, terrified, frightened, threatened, intimidated, anxious and hopeless) and was labeled Afraid. Component 3 consisted of 4 emotions (pleasure, optimistic, energetic and happiness) and was labeled Excited, while Component 4 had only two emotions (satisfied and joy) and was labeled Content. Component 5 had but a single emotion (aroused) and was labeled Aroused.

  Component
1                               2                            3                          4                           5
Unhappy Afraid Excited Content Aroused
uncomfortable 0.654 0.230 -0.036 -0.091 0.306
disturbed 0.704 0.140 -0.088 -0.101 0.217
worried 0.689 0.397 -0.044 -0.102 -0.014
satisfied -0.226 -0.047 0.235 0.798 -0.131
energetic -0.015 0.107 0.655 -0.068 0.455
happiness -0.259 0.043 0.633 0.331 -0.030
aroused 0.088 0.008 -0.072 0.051 0.854
joy -0.100 0.024 0.331 0.776 0.213
pleasure -0.080 -0.020 0.848 0.086 0.095
optimistic 0.003 0.000 0.758 0.324 -0.129
pessimistic 0.530 0.321 -0.023 -0.201 0.025
sadness 0.852 0.244 -0.148 -0.026 0.017
frustrated 0.872 0.204 -0.018 -0.028 -0.054
hopeless 0.473 0.527 -0.210 0.153 -0.194
intimidated 0.289 0.702 0.118 -0.059 -0.119
anger 0.815 0.218 -0.026 -0.002 0.010
scared 0.223 0.858 0.026 -0.072 -0.143
anxious 0.456 0.597 -0.132 0.155 0.137
fearful 0.224 0.866 -0.074 0.065 -0.005
threatened 0.133 0.798 0.183 -0.251 0.031
frightened 0.300 0.805 -0.018 0.035 0.258
terrified 0.223 0.844 0.028 0.060 0.172
irritated 0.681 0.160 -0.057 -0.171 -0.111

1Using the Varimax with Kaiser Normalization Rotation Method with the rotation converged in 6 iterations with a cut-off point set at 0.50 for component loadings.

Table 4: Principal Component Analysis1 (PCA) of the 23 emotions on the SOETUF Post-Trigger (P-T) Questionnaire for the combined subjects (n=83) in the both the film and text trigger groups of college students.

PCA component
groupings label
Trigger
Group
Mean
scores(±s.d.)
Statistical 
Significance
Statistical Significance
adjusted for sex
Unhappy Film
Text
23.2(±6.2)
21.3(±7.7)   
0.20      0.17
Afraid Film
Text
13.2(±8.2)
12.6(±8.7)  
0.75      0.75
Excited Film
Text
1.2(±1.9)
1.4(±1.8)   
0.69      0.64
Content Film
Text
0.4(±0.8)
0.1(±0.4)   
0.03** 0.03**
Aroused Film
Text
0.2(±0.5)
0.7(±1.1)  
0.01** 0.01**

** statistically significant

Table 5: Comparison of mean scores for the 5 identified PCA component groupings of the 23 emotions in the Emotional Reactions Domain of Interest (ER-DOI) between the film exposure (n=41) versus text exposure (n=42) trigger groups.

The only statistically significant findings among these five labeled PCA components between the film exposure trigger group and the text exposed trigger group were for the Content and the Aroused components (Table 5). For the Content component (which had a scale range of 0-8 as it was comprised of two emotions), while both groups scored it on the lower end of the scale, the text trigger group reported less more content, at a statistically significant level of 0.03. Conversely, for the Aroused component (which had a scale range of only 0-4 as it was comprised of only one emotion) the text trigger group reported being more aroused at a statistically significant level of 0.01. These observed statistical differences for the PCA components of Content and Aroused remained after adjusting for sex with females having lower scores on the Content component (0.08 ± 0.27 vs. 0.40 ± 0.88) and higher scores on the Aroused component (0.48 ± 0.91 vs. 0.40 ± 0.88) [21].

Discussion

This study confirmed that the SOETUF P-T Questionnaire could detect differences in bioethical judgments and emotions after exposure to film vs. text triggers in college students and identified a subset of five emotions within the 23 emotion reactions items that can be used to construct hypotheses for testing in future studies.

The SOETUF P-T Questionnaire did detect differences in both of the primary specific aims. In the Emotions Reaction Domain of Interest (ER-DOI), seven of the 23 listed emotions were identified at different levels of intensity between the film vs. text trigger groups (Table 3). In addition, the overall range of level of intensity for the 23 emotions were spread well over the 5-point scale ranging from mean scores of 0.1 to 3.2. Moreover, this spread over the emotion intensity scale appeared, on face content validity, to be both reasonable and appropriate given the horrific nature of the bioethical story that was the focus of both the film and the text triggers. Specifically, the seven emotions rated at very low intensity (i.e., a mean score of <0.7 for both film and text) were pleasure, joy, happiness, satisfied, optimistic, aroused and energetic, while the nine emotions rated at the other end of the intensity scale (with a mean score of >2.4) were disturbed, frustrated, sadness, anger, irritated, worried, uncomfortable, pessimistic, and hopeless.

For the other primary specific aim that addressed the Principles of Bioethics Domain of Interest 'POB-DOI', the text exposure trigger group reported a statistically significant lower score on the overall 'POB-DOI' (mean score of 5.1 vs. 7.3 for the film exposed group) indicating that the text exposed student group identified more serious breaches of bioethical principles than did the film exposed student group (Table 1). This overall difference detected in the 'POB-DOI' was due to two mathematical factors: 1) the result of a statistically significant 3.5-fold difference in the subset of two 'POB-DOI' questions on the principle of beneficence (mean scores of 0.7 and 1.2 for the film exposed group vs. respective scores of 0.2 and 0.7 for the text exposed group); and, 2) the overall consistent trend in which the text group had lower, albeit not statistically significant, scores for each of the six items on the three remaining principles of bioethics: autonomy, non-maleficence and justice.

Additionally, it is worthwhile to note that the overall mean 'POB-DOI' scores for both the text and film group (5.1 and 7.3, respectively, on a scale that scored from 0- 32) indicated the students’ judgment-regardless of trigger exposure-that the story violated bioethical principles, i.e., had 'POB-DOI' scores toward the lower end of the score range. Further it should be noted that while the SOETUF P-T Questionnaire did not detect any difference between the film trigger and the text trigger regarding the emotional power of the story, the sense of injustice aroused or in the sense of their morality having been violated, the mean scores for each of these three measures of impact were uniformly high, achieving mean scores ranging from 3.1-3.5 on a 4-point scale for both the film and text trigger groups (Table 2).

The Principal Component Analysis (PCA), conducted on the 23 items of the listed emotion reactions to achieve pattern recognition and data reduction in that set of 23 items for the overall SOETUF P-T Questionnaire for the combined film and text trigger groups, identified 5 components, or groupings, among those 23 emotions based on eigenvalues of less than one and inspection of the scree plot. The first grouping, which by PCA definition explains the most variance in the overall 23 emotion set of variables, was labeled as Unhappy consisted of eight emotions (in descending order: frustrated, sadness, anger, disturbed, worried, irritated, uncomfortable, and pessimistic). The second grouping, which then explained most of the remaining variance among the emotions, was labeled as Afraid also consisted of eight emotions (fearful, scared, terrified, frightened, threatened, intimidated, anxious and hopeless). The remaining three groupings, or components, labeled as Excited, Content and Aroused as described above in the results, then in descending order explained the remaining variance in the emotions data set (Table 4).

These five identified PCA components best fit the model of emotion groupings proposed by James A. Russell, a renowned research psychologist, who mapped emotion on a twodimensional plane, with positive-negative valence on one axis (pleasure versus displeasure) and high-low energy on the other (activation versus deactivation). Using the Russell mapping model, the unhappy component represents displeasure with lower activation, and the afraid component represents displeasure with higher activation. For the two positive components, Excited represents pleasure with higher activation, and Content represents pleasure with lower activation. While the remaining Aroused component is usually interpreted in the Russell Model as representing sexual energy, given that the subject matter that both the film and text trigger groups were exposed to was the horrific bioethical abuse story of the USPHS Syphilis Study at Tuskegee, aroused in this case might more likely be interpreted as being ‘aroused to action’.

Only the Content component and the Aroused component of these five PCA components were detected to be statistically different between the film versus text exposure trigger groups.

The mean scores for Content reveal that the text exposure group was much less likely to ‘be content’ after the exposure (a mean of 0.1 vs. a mean of 0.4) although both groups reported being at the lower end of the 8-point scale range. Thus, neither group was ‘content’ upon exposure, the text group less so. Conversely, for the Aroused component, the mean scores indicated that the text group was more aroused. Thus the two statistically significant differences detected in the five PCA components indicated that the text exposed trigger group was less content and more aroused than the film exposed trigger group. After adjusting for sex, both the Content and Aroused components remained statistically significantly different for the film versus text trigger groups.

The limitations of this study must be borne in mind as one tries to generalize from these findings, for this was the first use of the SOETUF P-T Questionnaire. All findings must be considered as ‘first look’ type data. Further, as the first application of the SOETUF P-T Questionnaire, the findings must be seen-at this very early stage of investigation-as being highly specific to the one Tuskegee Syphilis study documentary film used and the one Tuskegee Syphilis Study written text used as exposure triggers. Care, at this stage, must be taken not to generalize the findings of this pilot study to broadly hold for ‘films vs. text’ comparison until subsequent studies have explored that statement by using a variety of film and text triggers on bioethical emotions and principles. Finally, the unique nature of the college student body at the NYU Abu Dhabi campus must be keep in mind as a caution against generalizing the findings of this pilot study as the 83 student volunteers to serve as study subjects were attracted from a total student body where each entering class of ~300 comes from 75-85 different countries. This international student body is truly ‘global’ in background and culture, undoubtedly another factor to control in future research applications of the SOETUF P-T Questionnaire to enhance generalization to identified specific cultures. Finally, the last limitation is that as there have been no previous studies reporting on a direct comparison of reactions, emotional impact or bioethical principles learned between film vs. text triggers, we cannot compare our pilot findings to any prior published work, thus limiting our capability for putting our results into a broader context using comparative published literature.

Future plans for pursuing this line of investigation include administering the SOETUF P-T Questionnaire, using these same two specific trigger exposures, to geographically and culturally identifiable sets of high school students to explore and contrast the impact of ‘film vs. text’ on the teaching of bioethics to high school students across various cultures and regions. Other plans include introducing other films and other texts into the same research design to determine whether there are generalizable consistent patterns as to the reactions of high school students to film vs. text triggers.

Acknowledgement

Thanks to the following baccalaureate students at NYU Abu Dhabi who made significant contributions as members of the research team to the development of the SOETUF Questionnaires: Annie Bauer, Jacob Chagnon, and Peter Si, as well as to NYUAD faculty member Deepak Unnikrishnan.

References

  1. Alexander M, Lenahan P, Pavlov A (2005) Cinemeducation: a comprehensive guide to using film in medical education. Oxford: Radcliffe Publishing.
  2. Aleksandrova-Yankulovska S (2016) An innovative approach to teaching bioethics in management of healthcare. Nurs Ethics. 23:167-175.
  3. Champoux JE (2001) Using film to visualize principles and practices. 1st ed. Cincinnati, OH: South-Western College Publishing.
  4. https://campus.usal.es/~revistamedicinacine/numero_1/version_ingles/ing_1/ed_ing_1.pdf
  5. Giacalone RA,Juriewicz CL (2001)Lights, camera, action: teaching ethical decision making through the cinematic experience. Teaching Business Ethics. 5:79-87.
  6. Lumlertqul N, Kijpaisalratana N, Pityaratstian N, Waangsaturaka D (2009)  Cinemeducation: a pilot student project using movies to help students learn medical professionalism.  Med Teach. 31: e327-32.
  7. McAllister M, Levett-Jones T, Petrini MA, Lasater K (2016) The viewing room: A lens for developing ethical comportment.  Nursing Educ Pract.16:119-124.
  8. Pereira-Rates CM, Maciel SL, Moura PL, Minas G (2014) The use of film as a teaching tool for the teaching-learning process in bioethics. Invest Educ Enferm. 32:421-429.
  9. Ber R, Alroy G (2001) Twenty years of experience using trigger films as a teaching tool. Acad Med. 76:656-658.
  10. Schwartz B, Bohay R (2012) Can patients healp each professionalism and empathy to dental students?  Adding patient videos to a lecture course.  J Dent Educ. 76:174-84.
  11. Semendeferi I (2014) Feelings and ethics education: the film dear scientists. J Microbiol Biol Educ. 15:100-102.
  12. Semendeferi I, Tsiamyrtzis P, Dscosta M, Pavlidis L (2016) Connecting Past with Present: A Mixed-Methods Science Ethics Course and Its Evaluation. Sci Eng Ethics. 22: 251-74.
  13. Solberg LB, Freund TC. (2015). Teaching Bioethics at Historically Black Colleges and Universities. J Health Care Poor Underserved. 26:328-34.
  14. https://med.stanford.edu/medicineandthemuse/medethicsfilms/about.html
  15. Volandes A (2007) Medical ethics on film: towards a reconstruction of the teaching of healthcare professionals. J Med Ethics. 33:678-80.
  16. Woehlke S,Schicktanz S (2010) Movies as teaching material-ethical issues in organ transplantation. In:Wiesemann C,Wohlke S(eds) Teaching ethics in organ transplantation and tissue donation. Cases and movies.Gottingen:Universitatsverlag Gottingen.
  17. Self DJ, Baldwin DC (1990) Teaching medical humanities through film discussions. J Med Humanit. 11:23-29.
  18. Russell JA (2003) Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological Review. 110: 145-172.
  19. Brandt AM (1978) Racism and research: The case of the Tuskegee Syphilis Study. The Hastings Center Report. 8:21-29.
  20. Bad Blood (film), Diverse Productions, Ltd, England (1992).
  21. Self DJ, Baldwin Jr. DC, Olivarez M (1993) Teaching medical ethics to first-year students by using film discussion to develop their moral reasoning. Acad Med. 68:383-385.